FROM:
The Lancet 2011 (Feb 4); 377 (9764): 494–503 ~ FULL TEXT
Dr Lidy M Pelsser MSc • Klaas Frankena PhD • Jan Toorman MD • Prof Huub F Savelkoul PhD
Prof Anthony E Dubois MD • Rob Rodrigues Pereira MD • Ton A Haagen MD
Nanda N Rommelse PhD • Prof Jan K Buitelaar MD
Dr Lidy M Pelsser, ADHD Research Centre,
Liviuslaan 49, 5624 JE Eindhoven,
Netherlands
According to this new study, just published in Lancet Journal, a diet free of processed foods significantly reduces the symptoms of ADHD in 78% of 4-8 year old children. This 5-week study involving 100 subjects found that 63% of them experienced a relapse in ADHD symptoms upon re-introduction of problem foods into the diet.
Please review an in-depth review of this article at our Blog
You may also enjoy our ADD/ADHD Page
|
Background The effects of a restricted elimination diet in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) have mainly been investigated in selected subgroups of patients. We aimed to investigate whether there is a connection between diet and behaviour in an unselected group of children.
Methods The Impact of Nutrition on Children with ADHD (INCA) study was a randomised controlled trial that consisted of an open-label phase with masked measurements followed by a double-blind crossover phase. Patients in the Netherlands and Belgium were enrolled via announcements in medical health centres and through media announcements. Randomisation in both phases was individually done by random sampling. In the open-label phase (first phase), children aged 4—8 years who were diagnosed with ADHD were randomly assigned to 5 weeks of a restricted elimination diet (diet group) or to instructions for a healthy diet (control group). Thereafter, the clinical responders (those with an improvement of at least 40% on the ADHD rating scale [ARS]) from the diet group proceeded with a 4-week double-blind crossover food challenge phase (second phase), in which high-IgG or low-IgG foods (classified on the basis of every child's individual IgG blood test results) were added to the diet. During the first phase, only the assessing paediatrician was masked to group allocation. During the second phase (challenge phase), all persons involved were masked to challenge allocation. Primary endpoints were the change in ARS score between baseline and the end of the first phase (masked paediatrician) and between the end of the first phase and the second phase (double-blind), and the abbreviated Conners' scale (ACS) score (unmasked) between the same timepoints. Secondary endpoints included food-specific IgG levels at baseline related to the behaviour of the diet group responders after IgG-based food challenges. The primary analyses were intention to treat for the first phase and per protocol for the second phase. INCA is registered as an International Standard Randomised Controlled Trial, number ISRCTN 76063113.
Findings Between Nov 4, 2008, and Sept 29, 2009, 100 children were enrolled and randomly assigned to the control group (n=50) or the diet group (n=50). Between baseline and the end of the first phase, the difference between the diet group and the control group in the mean ARS total score was 23·7 (95% CI 18·6—28·8; p<0·0001) according to the masked ratings. The difference between groups in the mean ACS score between the same timepoints was 11·8 (95% CI 9·2—14·5; p<0·0001). The ARS total score increased in clinical responders after the challenge by 20·8 (95% CI 14·3—27·3; p<0·0001) and the ACS score increased by 11·6 (7·7—15·4; p<0·0001). In the challenge phase, after challenges with either high-IgG or low-IgG foods, relapse of ADHD symptoms occurred in 19 of 30 (63%) children, independent of the IgG blood levels. There were no harms or adverse events reported in both phases.
Interpretation A strictly supervised restricted elimination diet is a valuable instrument to assess whether ADHD is induced by food. The prescription of diets on the basis of IgG blood tests should be discouraged.
From the Full-Text Article:
Introduction
Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) aff ects
5% of children worldwide and is characterised by
excessive and impairing inattentive, hyperactive, and
impulsive behaviour. [1] Genetic and environmental factors
are involved, [2] and ADHD is often accompanied by
oppositional defiant disorder. [3] Children with ADHD
and comorbid oppositional defiant disorder are difficult
for parents, guardians, and teachers to handle, give rise
to substantial parenting stress, and have a worse
prognosis for adverse outcomes (ie, an increased risk of
developing conduct disorder and antisocial personality
disorder) than have children without comorbidity. [4] At
present, ADHD is treated with psychoeducation, parent
training, child behavioural interventions, and drugs, [5]
but follow-up studies have reported limited long-term
effects of multimodal treatment. [6, 7]
One of the risk factors for ADHD that could be
targeted for intervention is food. [8] Reports of adverse
physical reactions to foods (eg, eczema, asthma, and gastro intestinal problems) that affect various organ
systems [9] have led to the suggestion that foods might also
affect the brain, resulting in adverse behavioural effects. [10]
Colourings and preservatives might have some effect on
the behaviour of children with or without ADHD, but
additives do not cause ADHD. [2, 5, 11, 12]
An individually
constructed restricted elimination diet, which consists
of some hypoallergenic foods, might be effective for
treatment of ADHD. [8, 11] The rationale of this diet for
children with ADHD is to investigate whether ADHD is
triggered by foods — ie, to identify a hypersensitivity
reaction to foods. In a small randomised controlled trial
that investigated the effects of a restricted elimination
diet, [13] we reported statistically significant and
clinically relevant effects on ADHD and oppositional
defiant disorder.
In children with ADHD that is triggered by foods,
ADHD meets the criteria of hypersensitivity according to
allergy nomenclature. [14] Accordingly, we postulated that
ADHD might be an allergic or non-allergic hypersensitivity
disorder in some children. [15] IgE is implicated in
typical food allergies. In reactions to food that are not
mediated by IgE, assessment of IgG levels might be
useful, [15] and IgG blood tests are offered — especially in
complementary care [17] — with the aim of establishing a
relation between foods and ADHD. According to this
theory, eating foods that induce high IgG levels would
lead to a substantial behavioural relapse whereas eating
those that induce low IgG levels would not. However,
there is no evidence for the effectiveness of these tests. [18]
The primary aim of the Impact of Nutrition on Children
with ADHD (INCA) study was to investigate the effects
of a restricted elimination diet on behaviour in children
with ADHD. The secondary aim was to differentiate
between non-allergic and allergic mechanisms in food-induced
ADHD.
Methods
Participants
Children were recruited at medical health centres and
through media announcements in the Netherlands and
Belgium. Interested parents or guardians (hereafter called
parents) were provided with verbal and written
information about the study. Eligible children were
assessed for ADHD and comorbid disorders by a senior
paediatrician (JT) using a structured psychiatric interview
(SPI). Children were included if they had been diagnosed
with ADHD of any subtype. [1] Further inclusion criteria
were children’s age 4–8 years (sufficiently young to
maximise dietary compliance), and parents with adequate
knowledge of Dutch and who were motivated to follow a
5-week restricted elimination diet. Exclusion criteria were
children receiving drugs or behavioural therapy for
ADHD, children already following a diet, or family
circumstances that were likely to prevent completion of
the study. The presence of comorbid psychiatric disorders
was not a reason for exclusion.
The INCA study was approved by the medical ethics
committee of Wageningen University and by the
executive board and ethics committee of Catharina
Hospital Eindhoven. The parents of children who
participated in the trial provided written informed
consent before week 1 of the study.
Randomisation and masking
INCA consisted of two phases. The first phase was an
open-label phase with masked paediatrician measurements.
After the baseline assessment, eligible children
were randomly assigned to either a diet group or a control
group. Randomisation was individually done by random
sampling. Ten blocks of ten identical, sealed envelopes
containing concealed treatment codes were made by a
masked epidemiologist (KF) to prevent unbalanced
assignment of treatment over time. Parents randomly
picked and opened an envelope. Staff who recruited and
assessed patients were not involved in the procedure used
to generate group allocations.
Because the diet was individually tailored and
restricted, a reliable placebo diet was not possible, thus
parents and teachers could not be masked to group
allocation. Also, the researcher (LP) who provided expert
advice to parents and teachers during the diet period
could not be masked. Parents were instructed not to
reveal dietary information to the paediatrician (JT) who
did masked assessments. [19]
The second phase was a double-blind crossover food
challenge phase in the diet group. Eligible children from
the diet group were randomly assigned, by simple
sampling, to one of two challenge groups. Each group
was offered either three foods that induce low IgG levels
or three that induce high IgG levels in a crossover design.
The three foods within each group were selected by an
independent dietician who was masked to group
assignment. The researcher, paediatrician, parents, and
teachers were masked to IgG allocation. KF did the data
entry for both phases and was masked to the assigned
treatment.
Procedures
During the trial, we used four questionnaires to assess
outcome: the 18-item ADHD rating scale (ARS), [20] tenitem
abbreviated Conners’ scale (ACS), [21] strengths and
difficulties questionnaire (SDQ), [22] and SPI. [23] The ARS,
which is based on the diagnostic and statistical manual
of mental disorders part IV (DSM-IV) criteria for ADHD,
consists of nine inattention and nine hyperactivity and
impulsivity criteria, with a four-point scale (0=never [less
than once a week], 1=sometimes [several times a week],
2=often [once a day], and 3=very often [several times a
day]). Three measures were taken from the ARS: total
score (0–54), inattention score (0–27), and hyperactivity
and impulsivity score (0–27). The ACS, also a four-point
rating scale, covers hyperactivity, impulsivity, attention,
mood, and temper tantrums. The DSM-IV-based SPI was used to assess oppositional defiant disorder (with the
eight DSM-IV oppositional defiant disorder criteria) and
conduct disorder (with seven of the 15 DSM-IV conduct
disorder criteria relevant to this young group of patients —
ie, criteria 1–5, 9, and 11).
Table 1
|
The SDQ provides a total
difficulties score on the basis of the results of four
problem subscales: emotional symptoms, and conduct,
hyperactivity–inattention, and peer problems. Unmasked
parent and teacher assessments (ACS, ARS, and SPI)
and masked paediatrician assessments (ARS and SPI)
were done at baseline and at the end of the first phase
(week 9 in the diet group and week 13 in the control
group; table 1). The masked paediatrician based his
ratings on information obtained from the parents as well
as on his own observation and assessment of the child’s
behaviour and presentation. The masked measurements
were used for all analyses in the first phase, apart from
the ACS score and the week 9 measurements in the
control group. Blood samples were taken at the start and
end of the first phase.
After the baseline assessments, randomisation was
done, and parents started a 2-week baseline period during
which they did not exclude any foods from their child’s
diet. Parents kept extended diaries (containing
information on the child’s diet, behaviour, activities, physical complaints, and medications; webappendix p 1)
and closely monitored their child’s behaviour. After the
baseline period (in week 3), the second unmasked parent
assessment took place (ACS and ARS) and parents and
teachers filled in the SDQ.
During week 4 (start of the first phase), the diet group
started a 5-week individually designed restricted
elimination diet, which has been described elsewhere [24]
(webappendix p 2). Briefl y, the diet consisted of the few foods
diet (ie, rice, meat, vegetables, pears, and water) [8, 24]
complemented with specific foods such as potatoes,
fruits, and wheat. The aim was to create an elimination
diet as comprehensive as possible for each individual
child, to make the intervention easy for children and
their parents to follow. [10, 13] If the parents reported no
behavioural changes by the end of the second diet week,
the diet was gradually restricted to the few-foods diet
only. [10] At the end of the first phase, all children were
assessed by the masked paediatrician (ARS and SPI),
unmasked parent and teacher ratings (ACS, ARS, and
SPI) were done, the SDQ was completed by all parents
and teachers, and blood samples were taken. Children in
the diet group who had behavioural improvement of at
least 40% on the ARS—ie, clinical responders—entered
the challenge phase; the non-responders left the trial.
IgE and IgG levels were analysed from the blood
samples taken at week 1. Total IgE, food-specifi c IgE (to
chicken egg, peanut, soy, milk, fish, and wheat), and
food-specific total IgG levels to 270 different foods were
assessed with ELISA. Based on the levels of IgG (µg/mL)
in serum, measured with a certified IgG-specific food
screening test (ImuPro test), each analysed food was
categorised as a low-IgG food or a high-IgG food.
In the diet group responders, in the second phase
(double-blind crossover challenge phase; weeks 10–13),
two groups of foods consisting of either three high-IgG or
three low-IgG foods were consecutively added to the
restricted elimination diet, each for 2 weeks. For every
child, the composition of the food challenge groups was
tailored by the dietician on the basis of total IgG levels to
270 different foods, which were assessed in the first blood
samples. Any of the 270 foods could be chosen by the
dietician, except for foods that caused increased IgE levels
(to preclude an anaphylactic reaction), were disliked by the
child, or were already part of the diet. Thus, the foods
added in the challenge phase were individually chosen
and differed per child. All children were to complete both
challenges, and each challenge food group had to be eaten
every day in equal amounts during the 2-week period or
until behavioural changes occurred.
All behavioural measurements in the challenge
phase were double-blind. Parent ACS and ARS
assessments were done after each challenge; the other
measurements were done at week 13 or at week 11 if there
was a relapse in behaviour during the first challenge
(table 1). If the child’s behaviour showed no relapse
(according to the double-blind parent ARS score) during
the first challenge period (weeks 10–11), the child
proceeded with the second challenge (weeks 12–13), and a
third blood sample was taken at week 13. Conversely, if the
ADHD problems returned during the first challenge, the
third blood sampling was brought forward, after which
the challenge foods were eliminated again. After a washout
period, the length of which depended on the remission of
the behavioural problems, the second challenge would
start, after which the randomised controlled trial ended.
After the baseline period, the control group followed
the first phase until week 13 and received healthy food
advice according to the guidelines of the Dutch Nutrition
Centre. Parents continued to keep an extended diary until
the end of the trial (week 13). Measurements took place at
comparable times to the measurements in the diet group
(table 1). At week 13, the second blood sample was taken,
after which all parents of children who did not show
behavioural improvements were offered the possibility of
starting the diet.
The first phase primary endpoints were the difference in
ARS (masked paediatrician assessment) and ACS scores
(parent; unmasked assessment) between baseline and the
end of the first phase. The challenge phase primary
endpoints, in the clinical responders, were the change in
ARS and ACS score from the end of the first phase to
week 11 (after the first challenge) and week 13 (after the
second challenge). A relapse in ADHD behaviour was
defined as an ARS increase of at least 40% of the ARS score
at the end of the first phase, and up to at least 60% of the
ARS baseline score.
The first phase secondary endpoints were the IgE blood
levels at the start of the trial associated with the behavioural
changes at the end of the first phase, and the child’s
comorbid behavioural problems, assessed by the change in
SPI [13] scores (masked paediatrician) from week 1 and SDQ [22]
scores (parent) from week 3 to the end of the first phase.
The challenge phase secondary endpoints were the foodspecific IgG levels at baseline related to the behaviour of
the diet group responders after IgG-based food challenges.
The other secondary endpoints of physical and sleep
problems assessed with the other complaints questionnaire,
24 and other blood tests, as specified in the INCA
protocol, will be assessed in a separate paper.
Statistical analysis
In our previous randomised controlled trial, [13] 11 of
15 children in the diet group and none of 12 children in
the control group showed behavioural improvements of
40% or more. We therefore assumed that a behavioural
improvement of at least 40% would occur in 60% of
children in the diet group and in 20% of those in the
control group in this study. To achieve 80% power
(a=0·05, two sided test), taking into account a potential
block effect and 10% dropouts, we calculated that
40 children per group were needed. To allow for a
potentially higher percentage of dropouts, we included
ten extra children per group.
We did statistical analyses with Stata (version 10) and
SPSS (version 15). In the first phase, masked measurements
were done at Catharina Hospital Eindhoven by JT and
unmasked measurements were done at the ADHD
Research Centre Eindhoven by LP. In the second phase,
double-blind measurements were done by JT and LP. The first phase ARS and SPI analyses were done with the
masked measurements and were by intention to treat, last
observation carried forward. The challenge phase analyses
were per protocol. To assess the agreement between the
unmasked (parent) and masked paediatrician
measurements for ARS and SPI, we calculated kappa
values,25 and intra-cluster correlation coeffi cients (ICCs) [26]
for categorical and continuous parameters, respectively.
Kappa values greater than 0·75 (ICC >0·80) were taken to
represent excellent agreement beyond chance; values
below 0·40 (ICC <0·40) suggested poor agreement.
Behavioural endpoint scores were analysed by a
general linear model with treatment (diet group vs
control group), block, and their interaction as independent
variables and baseline scores as covariates. The
most reduced model was selected but treatment and
block were forced in each model. We assessed the fit of
the models with the link test command of Stata. The
association between clinical response (yes or no) and
treatment, and its association with IgE blood levels was
calculated with Fisher’s exact test. We analysed the effect
of the crossover challenges (low-IgG or high-IgG) on the
child’s behaviour with the Mainland-Gart procedure. [27]
We did a second analysis that also included those
children who responded equally to both challenges with
the Prescott test. [27] The effect of the challenges (low-IgG,
high-IgG) was expressed as odds ratios (ORs) and
estimated by generalised estimated equations (binomial
distribution, logit link), with adjustment for challenge
period and intra-patient correlation.
INCA is registered as an International Standard
Randomised Controlled Trial, number ISRCTN 76063113.
The protocol for this study was peer reviewed and
accepted by The Lancet; a summary of the protocol was
published on the journal’s website, and the journal
then made a commitment to peer review the primary
clinical manuscript.
Role of the funding source
The sponsors of the study had no role in the study
design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation,
writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to submit
for publication. All authors had full access to the data in
the study and LMP, NNR, and JKB had fi nal responsibility
for the decision to submit for publication.
Results
Figure 1
Table 3
Figure 2
|
Between Nov 4, 2008, and Sept 29, 2009, 100 children
were enrolled and randomly assigned to the control
group (n=50) or the diet group (n=50; Figure 1). Most
children were boys and the mean age was 6·9 years
(SD 1·3; Table 2). Of the 41 children in the diet group who
completed the first phase, the diet of 17 was restricted to
the few-foods diet only.
Table 3 and Figure 2 show the ARS results from the first
phase. Of the 41 (82%) of 50 children in the diet group
who completed the first phase, nine (22%) of 41 did not
and 32 (78%) of 41 did respond to the diet (figure 1). The
mean difference in ARS score between baseline and the
end of the first phase was significantly lower in the diet
group than in the control group for both the masked
paediatrician (p<0·0001) and unmasked teacher ratings
(p<0·0001; table 3). When comparing the unmasked
(parent; LP) with the masked (JT) ARS and SPI
measurements from the first phase, both kappa and ICC
of inter-rater agreement were greater than 0·40 (mean
0·90 [SD 0·07] for ICC and 0·83 [0·20] for kappa). The
ACS score between baseline and the first phase was also
significantly lower in the diet group than in the control
group for both parent (p<0·0001) and teacher (p<0·0001)
ratings (table 3).
The difference between groups on the oppositional
defiant disorder criteria measured by the SPI at the end of
the first phase was also significant for both the masked
paediatrician (p<0·0001) and teacher ratings (p=0·0320;
table 3; figure 2). Because only three children in the diet
group met the criteria for conduct disorder, we
did not analyse these results. The decrease in
hyperactivity–inattention problems, measured on the SDQ, was similar to the decrease on the ARS
(webappendix p 3).
Prespecified IgE immunological analyses in responders
(32 of 41) and non-responders (nine of 41) in the diet group
showed no association between clinical response and
increased IgE blood levels. Total IgE was increased in six
of 30 responders (data missing for two children) and two
of nine non-responders (p=1·0, Fisher’s exact test). Foodspecific IgE levels were increased in one of 31 responders
(data missing for one child) and one of nine nonresponders
(p=0·41, Fisher’s exact test).
Of the 32 children who were clinical responders,
30 proceeded to the challenge phase (figure 1). 19 of 30
showed a behavioural relapse after one or both
challenges. The ACS (unmasked parent) and ARS
(masked paediatrician) results in the children in the
diet group who were included in the challenge phase
(n=30) were compared with the results of the children
in the control group who completed the trial (n=42;
figure 3). The decrease in ARS total score in the clinical
responders from baseline to the end of the first phase
was 35·9 (95% CI 33·2–38·6; p<0·0001), which
subsequently increased after the challenge by 20·8
(14·3–27·3; p<0·0001). The decrease in ACS score in
the clinical responders from baseline to the end of the
first phase was 18·3 (95% CI 16·7–19·9; p<0·0001),
which increased after the challenge by 11·6 (7·7–15·4;
p<0·0001). In the control group, the ARS score did not
differ between the measurements at week 1 and
week 9 (0·8, 95% CI –0·4 to 2·0; p=0·21) and week 9
and week 13 (0·8, –0·4 to 2·0; p=0·17). In the control
group, the ACS score did not differ between week 1 and
week 9 (0·2, 95% CI –0·8 to 0·4; p=0·5) and between
week 9 and week 13 (0·2, –0·5 to 1·0; p=0·57). SDQ
measurements showed similar results (webappendix p 4).
Because only six of 30 teacher data were available at the
end of the second phase, we did not analyse
these results.
29 of 30 children were included in the IgG assessments
(no suitable high-IgG foods were available for one
responder; figure 1). 11 of 29 children were randomly
assigned to start with the low-IgG challenge and 18 to the
high-IgG challenge. Each challenge was followed by the
other challenge. 13 of 29 low-IgG challenges and 13 of
29 high-IgG challenges resulted in a relapse of ADHD
behaviour. No relapse was reported in 11 of 29 children,
eight had relapses after both challenges, 15 had relapses
after the first challenge, and 11 after the second challenge.
The sequence of the challenges (low-IgG then high-IgG
or high-IgG then low-IgG) was not significantly associated with the relapse of ADHD symptoms (Mainland-Gart
p=1·0; Prescott p=0·38). The generalised estimated
equations model showed no significant effects of IgG
type (high-IgG vs low-IgG OR 0·86, 95% CI 0·36–2·09;
p=0·75) or challenge period (first challenge
vs second challenge 0·55, 0·23–1·33; p=0·26). Parents,
teachers, and children reported no harms or adverse
events in the first or second phase.
Discussion
In the INCA study, the restricted elimination diet had a
significant beneficial effect on ADHD symptoms in
32 (64%) of 50 children, and reintroducing foods led to a
significant behavioural relapse in clinical responders.
Blood tests assessing IgG levels against foods did not
predict which foods might have a deleterious behavioural
effect. The effect of the diet was consistent and had a
similar effect in reducing both ADHD and oppositional
defiant disorder symptoms. Because of the worse
prognosis of children with comorbid oppositional defiant
disorder compared with those without comorbid disease,
interventions that reduce oppositional defiant disorder
symptoms have great clinical potential. The number of
children with conduct disorder was, in accordance with
the young age of the patients, too small to draw
conclusions.
Total IgE levels were increased only in a few children,
equally in responders and non-responders, suggesting
that the underlying mechanism of food sensitivity in
ADHD (which could be related to genetic factors [28]) is nonallergic,
although we cannot rule out the involvement of a
cell-mediated allergic response. In the second phase, some
eliminated foods were added to the diet of the responders.
Although the challenges consisted of only two groups of
three different individually selected foods, there was a
substantial relapse in behaviour in 63% of children. We
recorded no difference in behavioural effects after
challenge with high-IgG or low-IgG foods. These results
suggest that use of IgG blood tests to identify which foods
are triggering ADHD is not advisable. However, IgG blood
tests might be useful in other diseases. [29, 30]
Our results must be viewed in light of some limitations.
First, in the first phase, we did an open-label randomised
controlled trial with masked measurements by an
independent paediatrician because parents, teachers,
and researchers could not be masked. This method is
generally accepted and applied when a double-blind
randomised controlled trial cannot be done. [31-37]
Nevertheless, expectations of the parents cannot be fully
ruled out as a possible cause of the behavioural
improvements. Theoretically, the fact that the second
assessment was done by the paediatrician after 9 weeks
in the diet group compared with after 13 weeks in the
control group might have led to unmasking of the
paediatrician. To prevent this from happening, the
paediatrician was not informed about any previous
assessments. Because of the number of children
included, with new children starting every week, and
some children from the diet and control groups
returning every week for their second assessments, the
paediatrician was unlikely to remember whether he had
seen a particular child 9 or 13 weeks earlier. Parents
were also instructed not to reveal any information about
group assignment. Second, we cannot rule out that the
behavioural improvements during the first phase might
have been caused by increased attention for the child in
the diet group. However, to avoid differences between
groups the control group received healthy food advice
and parents kept an extended diary of their child’s behaviour during the trial. Furthermore, the relapse in
behaviour during the second phase, which required
comparable parental attention as in the first phase,
might be regarded as an internal replication of the
effects of the diet. Third, we applied a tailor-made diet
for each child to minimise the burden of the diet. In
24 (59%) of 41 children this individually composed diet
proved to be sufficient.
A strength of the INCA study was its design, which
included multiple ratings, its large sample size, and
blood tests to investigate the existence of an immunological
mechanism of action. Furthermore, the heterogeneous
sample is representative of the general
population of children with ADHD, and thus the results
of our study are applicable to young children with ADHD
whose parents are motivated to follow a 5-week dietary
investigation period (panel). Another strength is the
investigation of the effects of the diet on comorbid
disorders such as oppositional defiant disorder. The
results of the multiple ratings are consistent, which
provides evidence for the clinically relevant beneficial
effects of a restricted elimination diet on ADHD and
oppositional defiant disorder.
The mechanisms and effects of food need to be
investigated — eg, at a functional and structural brain
level and in relation to genetic factors that increase the
susceptibility to ADHD. Also, the challenge procedure,
which is done to identify the incriminated foods in clinical responders, should be made as easy as possible
to follow, to increase the feasibility of the diet. Furthermore,
the long-term effects of foods should be
investigated; children might outgrow the sensitivity to
the incriminating foods when they are avoided for a
long period of time.
Our study shows considerable effects of a restricted
elimination diet in an unselected group of children with
ADHD, with equal effects on ADHD and oppositional
defiant disorder. Therefore, we think that dietary
intervention should be considered in all children with
ADHD, provided parents are willing to follow a diagnostic
restricted elimination diet for a 5-week period, and
provided expert supervision is available. Children who
react favourably to this diet should be diagnosed with
food-induced ADHD and should enter a challenge
procedure, to define which foods each child reacts to, and
to increase the feasibility and to minimise the burden of
the diet. In children who do not show behavioural
improvements after following the diet, standard
treatments such as drugs, behavioural treatments, or
both should be considered.